Zimbardo's Stanford Prison Experiment

Share:

 

An important social research project is the Stanford Prison Experiment. This project is a classic example on how social institutions, irregardless of its forms and manner, impose rules and regulations to ensure conformity. However, like most of social research, there are several questions that abound, including limitations and ethical considerations. Due to the fact that social research projects are focused on people, certain level of attention and care is very important to avoid harming on the projects' participants, especially in an social research experiment.

Psychologist Philip Zimbardo, in 1971, created the Stanford prison experiment, a simulation of prison life, to understand and review the prisoner-guard interactions (Babbie, 2012). In this controversial social research, Zimbardo looks at the different power of roles between two interacting parties – prisoners and guards. It sees how the interaction between two powers or roles define the party's behavior, attitude, and change of personality. 

After setting up a  mock prison in one of Stanford University's buildings, Zimbardo selected participants  from the students of the university.  Zimbardo ran an advertisement in the university for individuals who are willing to play as prisoner or guards in an experiment that would last two weeks. Initially, there were 75 individuals who responded to the advertisements, but only twenty-four healthy male participants were admitted to the experiment. These individuals agreed to be under surveillance, harassed, but not to be physically abused for a compensation of $15 per day.

 

In determining the roles or conditions on which the participant would be subjected, a simple flip of the coin was used (Sheets-Johnstone, 2010). Participants wore appropriate uniforms that suited their roles. Prisoner-participants wore the prison uniform and were referred by their serial numbers, instead of their names. On the other hand, guard-participants wore khaki uniforms and dark sunglasses; they were also allowed to carry a symbolic club.  The rule of the Stanford Prison Experiment is simple. Guards were given authority to impose rules for control of the prison, and to punish violators of these rules thereof. In sense, there are no actual limits of rules. Guards were allowed to do whatever they want, either harassing or monitoring prisoners, but not to the extent of physical abuse.

Central to the experiment is to clearly demonstrate and present the power of situational forces and the effect on the behavior of normal and healthy participants – on both groups. It further reviews the essence of institutional power to the authority power of blind obedience.  With all this focus, the experiment, which was supposed to run two weeks, was called off in six days after an observer voiced criticism and necessity of addressing psychological impact on participants.

The results of the experiment were both stunning and disheartening. Participants already acted to their roles, being attached to it that they forgot they were just volunteers of the experiment. Guard-participants were abusive and tyrannical in dealing with prisoners who also suffered from depression and trauma (Wildman, 2007). With regards to Zimbardo and his team, they were also so occupied with roles as administrators and wardens of the prison that they failed to see the harm of the experiment.

At the end of the experiment, Zimbardo had to have all the participants debriefed and counseled to avoid the negative lasting impact of the experiment. However, there are several questions that still abound. How well did Zimbardo consider the ethical limitations of the study to avoid harm of participants? Were researchers able to point possible implications of the experiment to the participants?

Perhaps, one of the benefits that the Stanford Prison Experiment is that most universities set-up their own oversee committees. It limits the power of  the researcher over the power of the experiment that are harmful on the participants. Due to the fact that social research can alter the behavior and personality of the participants, the oversee committee checks the different aspects of the research on the behavior of individuals.

 

Yet, there are still different issues that should be sorted out with the experiment. First, there were no actual limits of the experiment. The rules of the experiment were too vague that it provided a leeway on the actions of the guard-participants. In essence, the experiment was not scientifically carried – there were no actual hypotheses tested but a general observation of behaviors and responses only. With these general and vague experiment hypotheses, as a result, control and management was not proper. Even if Zimbardo played the superintendent role, he failed to impose proper rules and control over the experiment, which made the entire Stanford Prison Experiment faulty and unethical.

 

Being a superintendent of the prison, in the experiment, Zimbardo had a wide control over the experiment's procedure. He could have issued several memorandums to re-dress the situation of experiment. As long as the researcher has a role in the experiment, it is easier for him or her to supervise and amend major ethical concerns that affect the total safety of participants.

In form, the experiment simulated the actual prison conditions, but in substance, the experiment failed to reconcile with the actual prison experience. Due to this fact, Baofu (2011) pointed out questions on the validity of the experiment based on ecological grounds. Even if Zimbardo argued that prison conditions are dehumanizing in sense and in form of making the dehumanization any act will suffice, however, it reduces the experiment to arbitrary observation only. Dehumanization is a well-guarded concern of any social project because it involves long-lasting impact on behavior of participants.

In the aim to arrive at a certain conclusion of the experiment, Zimbardo failed to recognize the harmful effects of the project. In fact, it took an outside observer to point the need to stop the experiment (Sheets-Johnstone, 2010). It took only two days for the participants to de-individualize and act on the roles they are playing. At same time, the prisoners already suffered tremendous depression and trauma that rebellion happened. Yet, Zimbardo still held to the time schedule of the experiment to arrive at the conclusion he wanted.

 

According to Baofu (2011), the study violated the Ethics Code of the American Psychological Association, the Canadian Code of Conduct of Research Involving Humans, and the Belmont Report. Baofu further noted that should the experiment be done right now, it would be impossible to get an approval.  Instead of stopping the experiment when the level of emotional and psychological harm was still minimal, Zimbardo proceeded with the experiment without regard to the mental health of the participants.

 

In any social project involving humans, the priority is the safety of the participants. The researcher can proceed with the experiment, only provided that safety is taken into account. Zimbardo could have stopped the experiment at once to avoid further harm on participants, which was resolved through psychological debriefing and counseling after the experiment was stopped.

Although the Zimbardo's Stanford Prison Experiment has good intentions to understanding the power of roles in prisons to resolve reports on inhuman treatments and wrong handling of prisoners, yet the experiment itself provided its own evils by overstepping on ethical standards. There are possible ways to conduct an experiment, or a social research, but the primary priority should always be the safety of participants. When these ethical considerations are not taken into account, the result of the experiment, even if it's promising and exciting, is easily eclipsed and discarded.

 

References:

 

Babbie, E. (2012). The Practice of Social Research. Cengage Learning.

 

Baofu, P. (2011). Beyond Ethics to Post-Ethics: A Preface to a New Theory of Morality and Immorality (Hc). IAP.

 

Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2010).  The Roots of Morality. Penn State Press.

 

Stolley, K. (2005). The Basics of Sociology. Greenwood Publishing Group.

 

Wildman, M. (2007). Inmate Deviant Behaviors: An Application of Tittle's Control Balance Theory. ProQuest.